
A figure from the research shows the same section of neural dendrite at days 1, 5 and 10 of imaging. Arrows indicate spines that will be lost by the next imaging day, or that were added by that day. Circle colors indicate which eye(s) the input came from. Credit: Sur Lab/MIT Picower Institute
Scientists have long known that the brain’s visual system isn’t fully hardwired from the start—it becomes refined by what babies see—but the authors of a new study funded by the National Eye Institute still weren’t prepared for the degree of rewiring they observed when they took a first-ever look at the process in mice as it happened in real-time.
As the researchers in The Picower Institute for Learning and Memory at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology tracked hundreds of “spine” structures housing individual network connections, or “synapses,” on the dendrite branches of neurons in the visual cortex over 10 days, they saw that only 40% of the ones that started the process survived. Refining binocular vision (integrating input from both eyes) required numerous additions and removals of spines along the dendrites to establish an eventual set of connections.
Former graduate student Katya Tsimring, Ph.D., led the study, which the team said is the first in which scientists tracked the same connections all the way through the “critical period,” when binocular vision becomes refined. Their findings published in Nature Communications.
The study involved imaging the same dendrites on the same neurons repeatedly over 10 days in the same live mouse through a critical period of development, to ask, what happens to the synapses or spines on them, said senior authors Mriganka Sur, Ph.D., and Lilah Newton, Ph.D., professors in The Picower Institute and MIT’s Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences. “We were surprised by how much change there is.”
Extensive turnover
In the experiments, young mice watched as black and white gratings with lines of specific orientations and directions of movement drifted across their field of view. At the same time, the scientists observed both the structure and activity of the neurons’ main body (or, “soma”) and of the spines along their dendrites. By tracking the structure of 793 dendritic spines on 14 neurons at roughly day 1, day 5 and day 10 of the critical period, they could quantify the addition and loss of the spines and therefore the synaptic connections they housed. And by tracking their activity at the same time, they could quantify the visual information the neurons received at each synaptic connection. For example, a spine might respond to one specific orientation or direction of grating, several orientations, or may not respond at all. Finally, by relating a spine’s structural changes across the critical period to its activity, they sought to uncover the process by which synaptic turnover refined binocular vision.
Structurally, the researchers saw that 32% of the spines evident on day 1 were gone by day 5, and that 24% of the spines apparent on day 5 had been added since day 1. The period between day 5 and day 10 showed similar turnover: 27% were eliminated but 24% were added. Overall, only 40% of the spines seen on day 1 were still there on day 10.
Meanwhile, only 4 of the 13 neurons they were tracking that responded to visual stimuli still responded on day 10. The scientists don’t know for sure why the other 9 stopped responding, at least to the stimuli they once responded to, but it likely they now served a different function.
What are the rules?
Having beheld this extensive wiring and rewiring, the scientists then asked what entitled some spines to survive over the 10-day critical period.
Previous studies have shown that the first inputs to reach binocular visual cortex neurons are from the “contralateral” eye on the opposite side of the head (so in the left hemisphere, the right eye’s inputs get there first), Sur said. These inputs drive a neuron’s soma to respond to specific visual properties such as the orientation of a line—for instance, a 45-degree diagonal. By the time the critical period starts, inputs from the “ipsilateral” eye on the same side of the head begin joining the race to visual cortex neurons, enabling some to become binocular.
It’s no accident that many visual cortex neurons are tuned to lines of different directions in the field of view, Sur said.
“The world is made up of oriented line segments,” Sur noted. “They may be long line segments; they may be short line segments. But the world is not just amorphous globs with hazy boundaries. Objects in the world—trees, the ground, horizons, blades of grass, tables, chairs—are bounded by little line segments.”
To read more, visit The Picower Institute